Hence, although positive outcomes were rewarding, it was only thr

Hence, although positive outcomes were rewarding, it was only through accurately estimating outcome delivery time that subjects could themselves exert a degree of control over their future payment. Subject’s mean time estimate

on instrumental test trials was close to the mean CS-US interval of 6 s (6.03 s ± 0.09 grand average over all test trials; 5.85 s ± 0.11 in test trials with variable timing CS; 6.22 s ± 0.09 in test trials with fixed timing CS), showing that participants had acquired an accurate representation of outcome timings and exploited the most rewarding policy. Caspase inhibitor Average timing estimates did not differ significantly from 6 s (p > 0.7 across all test trials). As expected, in test trials with fixed timing CS, time estimates were less variable than AZD2281 cost in trials with variable timing CS (Kolmogorov-Smirnov test: p < 0.001, k = 0.23; see Figure S1 available online). Furthermore, time estimates were on average shorter in variable timing compared to fixed timing trials (t27 = 5.27, p < 0.001; Table S1). After careful preprocessing steps to minimize effects of subject motion and physiological artifacts (see Experimental Procedures and Figure S2), we identified a midbrain region in the vicinity of the VTA using a functional contrast. Our aim here was to test whether the VTA BOLD

response coded for reward prediction errors in the fixed timing trials, and whether these responses were modulated by outcome time in variable timing trials. Consequently, we chose to identify the VTA using a contrast that was orthogonal to both these effects of interest and in so doing we avoided a potential selection bias. We contrasted unexpected rewards against unexpected zero outcomes in of the variable timing trials, averaged across delivery times, in an anatomically restricted region of interest (ROI) around VTA (see Experimental Procedures). Using this ROI, we proceeded to test whether the VTA response for fixed trials showed the hallmarks of reward prediction error activity. Consistent

with the profile seen in dopaminergic single unit recordings, we found that the BOLD response to the CS increased in proportion to the predicted reward magnitude of the trial (t test on regression slopes: t27 = 1.77; p = 0.05; pairwise one-tailed comparisons: 0p versus 0/40p: t27 = −2.44, p = 0.01; 0p versus 40p: t27 = −4.19, p < 0.001; 0/40p versus 40p: t27 = −2.47, p = 0.01), whereas the BOLD response to the US showed a marked increase for unexpected rewards (t27 = 4.30, p < 0.001, main effect of 40p US in 50:50 trials), and a difference between unexpected positive and zero outcomes (one-tailed t test: 40p versus 0p US in 50:50 trials: t27 = 1.75, p = 0.046; Figure 2). Next, we investigated VTA responses to variable CS-US timings.

Therefore, in the absence of TNFα,

Therefore, in the absence of TNFα, BAY 73-4506 purchase ML astrocytes maintain the same number of functional glutamate transporters and take up the same amount of glutamate as in

WT preparations, excluding astrocytic glutamate uptake as direct target of the TNFα-dependent control of gliotransmission. If P2Y1R stimulation fails to increase mEPSC frequency at GC synapses in Tnf−/− slices because of competition between defective astrocyte glutamate release and uptake, then blockade of the latter could be compensatory and artificially restore the synaptic effect lost by the defective release and, thereby, unmask its occurrence. To directly test this possibility, we added TBOA to hippocampal slices from Tnf−/− mice. At first, we checked the effects of the uptake blocker on basal activity in GCs. When applied at a subsaturating concentration (25 μM) ( Brasier and Feldman, 2008), TBOA produced a clear change in mEPSC activity, with no other detectable effect on membrane currents (which were instead affected using 100 μM of the uptake blocker, data not shown). Notably, TBOA selectively increased the frequency of mEPSC events (+52% ± 13%; p < 0.001, n = 10 cells; Figure 6A), without changing their amplitude and kinetics. This effect was fully reversed by ifenprodil ( Figure 6A), indicating that enhanced ambient glutamate activates pre-NMDAR. We then tested

the effect of P2Y1R stimulation in the presence of the Astemizole uptake blocker. Despite the TBOA-induced enhancement of basal mEPSC frequency in GCs, subsequent application Epigenetic inhibitor chemical structure of 2MeSADP in the continued presence of TBOA, produced a further significant increase in frequency (+47% ± 9%; p < 0.001; n = 18 cells) without changing amplitude

of the miniature events ( Figure 6B). Blocking NMDA receptors with ifenprodil in the presence of both 2MeSADP and TBOA fully reversed the overall increase in mEPSC frequency produced by the two agents ( Figure 6B). These data indicate that, in Tnf−/− mice, inhibition of glutamate uptake by TBOA produces two effects on mEPSC frequency in GCs, both mediated by activation of presynaptic NR2B-containing NMDARs: (1) an increase in the basal frequency of the events and (2) a further 2MeSADP-dependent increase, resembling the effect produced by the P2Y1R agonist in WT mice. Taking all our data together, the most plausible explanation for the latter effect is that, in situ like in culture, astrocytic glutamate release is deregulated in the absence of TNFα. Thus, although astrocytic glutamate in Tnf−/− slices fails to induce pre-NMDAR-dependent modulation, reappearance of this type of effect in the presence of TBOA confirms that the transmitter is indeed released upon P2Y1R stimulation, but defectively and, because of competing uptake, it does not reach an extracellular concentration sufficient to activate pre-NMDAR.

2) (Figure 4A, right) Both numerically (Figure 4B) and geometric

2) (Figure 4A, right). Both numerically (Figure 4B) and geometrically (Figure S3A), we confirmed that ΔVm1/ΔVm2 > Ge1/Ge2 (with Ge2 > Ge1) held true for all the model inputs. Such a “compression” effect has a great impact on stimulus selectivity of neuronal responses. Imagine that Ge2 and Ge1 represent the excitatory inputs evoked by the optimal and null stimuli, respectively. The selectivity existing in the excitatory inputs, as reflected by the ratio of Ge2 to Ge1, is greatly attenuated when the inputs are transformed into PSP responses. Since Ge can represent an input evoked by any type of physical stimulus,

such attenuation of tuning selectivity poses a ubiquitous problem for any feature-specific Selleckchem SAHA HDAC neuronal responses. To test how inhibition sharpens the blurred selectivity, we incorporated in the model an inhibitory input which followed the excitatory input with a temporal delay (50 ms) and whose conductance was the same as that of the excitatory input (1× inhibition), or double (2×), or triple (3×) that of the excitatory input.

As shown by the colored curves in Figure 4A, the presence of the inhibitory input slows down the saturation of PSP responses, and greatly expands the input dynamic range (Figure S3B), i.e., the range of excitatory input Endocrinology antagonist strengths that can be faithfully represented. With this altered input-output function, the ratio between the PSP amplitudes (ΔVm′1/ΔVm′2) became much closer to that between the initial input strengths (Ge1/Ge2). We also confirmed that over the physiological range of excitatory conductances, ΔVm′1/ΔVm′2 was always smaller than ΔVm1/ΔVm2 (Figures 4C and S3C),

indicating that mafosfamide inhibition effectively ameliorated the attenuation of tuning selectivity caused by the membrane filtering. To further illustrate the inhibitory effect on OS, we modeled excitatory and inhibitory inputs with their tuning profiles taken from experimental data, and simulated PSP responses resulting from excitatory inputs alone and from integrating excitatory and inhibitory inputs (Figure 4D). Similar as observed earlier (Figure 3D), the PSP tuning was largely flattened when only excitatory inputs were present (Figure 4D, top middle). To derive the tuning of spiking responses, we first used a threshold-linear model (Carandini and Ferster, 2000; see Experimental Procedures). Due to the blurred tuning selectivity of PSP responses which were all suprathreshold (Figure 4D, top middle, inset), the spiking response tuning exhibited only a weak bias with an OSI (= 0.18) much lower than observed experimentally (Figure 4D, top right). On the other hand, the presence of inhibition led to a sharper tuning of PSP responses (Figure 4D, bottom middle). In the meantime, inhibition suppressed many responses to off-optimal stimuli below the spike threshold.

Thus, finding support for the imitation hypothesis Our findings

Thus, finding support for the imitation hypothesis. Our findings seem to suggest that young see more adults behave in a particular way because their social environment passively evokes certain behaviors and less because they are actively or explicitly encouraged to behave in a specific way. Thus, our results may imply that passive peer influence may be of more importance

to understand young adult smoking than active peer influence. Our findings must be carefully interpreted but seem to suggest that smoking cessation programs and policy should probably target and put more emphasis on passive peer influence (rather than active peer influence) in order to decrease smoking among daily smoking young adults. There may be three possible ways they could address this. First of all, most of the smoking cessation campaigns portray smoking models in their ads which in themselves may induce people to smoke and may therefore be counterproductive. Therefore, smoking models should perhaps no longer be depicted in these campaigns. Second, interaction with smoking models should be prevented. Government policy has been contributing to this goal by restricting smoking in public settings (e.g., trains, airplanes, bars, restaurants). However, smoking is, surprisingly,

not yet officially banned in schoolyards worldwide; one of these countries Fulvestrant manufacturer that does not have such legislation in place is The Netherlands. We would

recommend stricter school policies in this respect for these countries (Griesbach et al., 2002, Schnohr et al., 2008 and Wold et al., 2004). Third, awareness should be increased of the urge to imitate others. Especially young adults trying to quit or reduce smoking need to be alerted to the effects of smoking by others in their presence, and to successfully quit or reduce smoking they should learn to avoid these situations. Smoking cessation campaigns could emphasize and support this message. Nevertheless, future studies are needed to replicate our science study to find support for our findings and to gain more knowledge on these two kinds of peer influences. There are several aspects that need to be taken into account in future research. First, we operationalized peer pressure as the verbal and nonverbal encouragement to take and smoke a cigarette but we did not take into account the possibility that in real life situations, this could be accompanied by teasing, taunting and rejection when the offered cigarette is declined. Although there is less evidence for the occurrence of this so-called coercive pressure (Arnett, 2007), future studies nevertheless need to examine its impact on student smoking. Second, more insights are needed on who are more likely to being imitated (e.g., popular peers), who are more likely to imitate (e.g.

While recent results thus support the existence of massive long-d

While recent results thus support the existence of massive long-distance cortical networks involving PFC and their role in conscious perception, two points should be stressed. First, the PFC is increasingly being decomposed into multiple specialized and lateralized subnetworks (e.g., Koechlin et al., 2003 and Voytek and Knight, 2010). These findings need not, however, be seen as contradicting the GNW hypothesis that these subnetworks, through their tight interconnections, interact so strongly as to make any information coded in one area quickly available to all others. Second, in addition to PFC, the

nonspecific thalamic nuclei, the Lapatinib basal ganglia, and some cortical nodes are likely to contribute to global information broadcasting (Voytek and Knight, 2010). The precuneus, in particular, may also operate as a cortical “hub” with a massive degree of interconnectivity (Hagmann et al., 2008 and Iturria-Medina et al., 2008). This region, plausibly homologous to the highly connected macaque posteromedial cortex (PMC) (Parvizi et al., 2006), is an aggregate of convergence-divergence zones (Meyer and Damasio, 2009) and is tightly connected to PFC area 46 and other workspace regions (Goldman-Rakic, 1999). In humans, the PMC may play a critical role in humans in self-referential processing (Cavanna and Trimble,

2006, Damasio, 1999 and Vogt and Laureys, 2005), thus allowing any conscious content to be integrated into a subjective first-person AUY-922 nmr perspective. NMDA receptors and GNW simulations. GNW simulations assume that long-distance bottom-up connections primarily impinge on fast glutamate

AMPA receptors while top-down ones primarily concern the slower glutamate NMDA receptor. This assumption contributes importantly to the temporal dynamics of the model, particularly the separation between a fast phasic bottom-up phase and a late sustained integration phase, mimicking experimental observations. It can be criticized as both receptor types are known to be present in variable proportions at glutamatergic synapses (for pioneering data on human Endonuclease receptor distribution, see Amunts et al., 2010). However, in agreement with the model, physiological recordings suggest that NMDA antagonists do not interfere with early bottom-up sensory activity, but only affect later integrative events such as the mismatch negativity in auditory cortex ( Javitt et al., 1996). Thus, although GNW simulations adopted a highly simplified anatomical assumption of radically distinct distributions of NMDA and AMPA, which may have to be qualified in more realistic models, the notion that NMDA receptors contribute primarily to late, slow, and top-down integrative processes is plausible (for a related argument, see Wong and Wang, 2006).

, 1996) ITS1, situated between the conserved

5 8S and 18

, 1996). ITS1, situated between the conserved

5.8S and 18S genes encoding the ribosomal RNA subunits, occurs in approximately 100–200 copies per genome of a trypanosome. Due to variation in sizes of ITS1 amongst different selleck Trypanosoma taxa, discrimination between species or subgenus is possible in a single run ( McLaughlin et al., 1996 and Desquesnes et al., 2001). ITS1 or nested ITS1/ITS2-based PCR assays have proven useful in trypanosomosis diagnosis and in epidemiological studies ( Njiru et al., 2005, Cox et al., 2005, Thumbi et al., 2008, de Clare Bronsvoort et al., 2010 and Fikru et al., 2012). The authors claimed that the universal ITS-based PCR assays reduce cost and time of running several species-specific assays, especially

in large-scale studies. ITS1 PCR that was used in an epidemiological survey in Ethiopia revealed a five-fold higher detection rate for T. vivax compared to HCT ( Fikru et al., 2012). However, evaluation of the assay as a test of cure has not been reported and these ITS1 PCR assays are prone to non-specific amplification, particularly with bovine blood (unpublished observations). Therefore, the assay presented in this study was further refined for optimal performance. “Touchdown” PCR approach, which employs more stringent primer-template hybridisation temperatures, was introduced to enhance assay specificity. In Touchdown PCR, the annealing temperature during the first PCR cycles is well above the predicted optimal annealing temperature of the primers thus favouring the amplification of the specific target sequence. In the following PCR cycles, the annealing temperature is gradually

Ruxolitinib in vitro lowered to more permissive temperatures. By maintaining the same high number of amplification cycles as in a classical PCR, the sensitivity is not compromised ( Don et al., 1991 and Korbie and Mattick, 2008). The objectives of this study were (1) to develop an ITS1 “Touchdown” PCR for multi-taxon detection of the Trypanosoma genus and (2) to evaluate the performance of this ITS1 TD PCR as test of cure in an efficacy study designed to evaluate novel trypanocidal compounds in cattle infected with T. congolense. Animal second studies at the Institute of Tropical Medicine (ITM, Antwerp, Belgium) received ethical clearance from the Veterinary Ethics Committee at ITM (BMW 2012-1 and BMW 2013-7). Animal studies at ClinVet (Bloemfontein, South Africa) received ethical approval from the ClinVet Animal Ethics Committee (CAEC) authorising the research facility to conduct three studies CV12/884; CV 12/928; CV12/885. Animals were housed and cared for in accordance with national and international legislation, and local animal regulatory requirements. The following T. congolense strains were used for infection of cattle: KONT 2/133, KONT 2/151 ( Mamoudou et al., 2008), Maputo 31J, Maputo 02J (unpublished). For test development, the following strains were used for infection of mice: T.

The upper marker of the heavy exercise intensity domain is the ma

The upper marker of the heavy exercise intensity domain is the maximal lactate steady state (MLSS, the highest metabolic rate at which exercise can be sustained without an accumulation of blood lactate33) or, more often in young people, the critical power (CP, the highest metabolic rate at which V˙O2 can be stabilised below peak V˙O236 and 37). Exercise above MLSS or CP but below

peak V˙O2 is in the very heavy exercise domain and exercise above peak V˙O2 is in the severe exercise domain.38 With young participants it has been noted that small selleck compound breath-to-breath variations are inherent to children’s response profiles.39 This reduces the confidence with which pV˙O2 kinetic responses can be estimated and confidence intervals are likely to be beyond acceptable limits unless sufficient identical transitions are aligned and averaged to improve the signal to

noise ratio.40 Rigorously determined and interpreted data from young people are available in the moderate, heavy and very heavy intensity exercise domains.41, 42 and 43 The pV˙O2 response to a step transition has three phases. At the onset there is an immediate increase in cardiac output which occurs prior to the arrival at the lungs of venous blood from the exercising muscles. This cardiodynamic phase (phase I) which, in children, lasts ∼15 s is independent of V˙O2 at the muscle (mV˙O2) and reflects an increase in pulmonary Ku-0059436 cost blood flow with exercise. Phase II, the primary component, is a rapid exponential increase in pV˙O2 that arises with hypoxic and hypercapnic blood from the exercising muscles arriving at the lungs. Phase II kinetics are described by the time constant (τ  ) which is the time taken unless to achieve 63% of the change in pV˙O2. In phases I and II ATP re-synthesis cannot be fully supported by oxidative phosphorylation and the additional energy requirements of the exercise are met from body oxygen stores, PCr and glycolysis. During moderate intensity exercise with children pV˙O2 reaches a steady state (phase III) within about 2 min. In the heavy intensity exercise domain, the primary phase II oxygen cost is similar to that observed during moderate

intensity exercise but the overall oxygen cost of exercise increases over time as a slow component of pV˙O2 is superimposed upon the primary component and the achievement of a steady state might be delayed by ∼10–15 min. 44 In adults, at exercise intensities above the MLSS or CP the slow component of pV˙O2 rises rapidly over time and eventually reaches peak V˙O2 but this phenomenon has not been observed in children. 37 and 45 The mechanisms underlying the pV˙O2 slow component remain speculative but it has been established that ∼86% have been accounted for at the contracting muscles.46 During exercise above the TLAC the pV˙O2 slow component is associated with a progressive recruitment of additional type II muscle fibres with the low efficiency contributing to the increased oxygen cost of exercise.

, 2003, Luk et al , 2009, Murray et al , 2003 and Serpell et al ,

, 2003, Luk et al., 2009, Murray et al., 2003 and Serpell et al., 2000). Thus, we asked whether human α-syn pffs composed of α-syn-1-120, α-syn-1-89, α-syn-58-140, or α-syn-NAC could seed formation of LBs and LNs in neurons. We observed that α-syn-1-120 and α-syn-1-89 pffs induced robust accumulation of endogenous p-α-syn aggregates that were Tx-100-insoluble (Figure 2A; data not shown), and they are morphologically indistinguishable from those formed by α-syn-hWT pffs. α-syn-58-140 pffs also seeded formation of endogenous mouse α-syn aggregates that were hyperphosphorylated

check details (Figure 2A). Moreover, pffs composed of only the central hydrophobic α-syn-NAC domain also resulted in endogenous mouse α-syn fibrillar LB-like aggregates that were Tx-100-insoluble. Overall, our data demonstrate that α-syn pffs containing only the central, hydrophobic portion of α-syn-hWT are sufficient to seed conversion of endogenous α-syn into pathological aggregates. Mice typically do not develop LBs except in the case of transgenic lines overexpressing mutant human α-syn. We thus asked whether the formation of LB-like aggregates required human α-syn or whether they can be seeded by α-syn

pffs generated from recombinant mouse WT α-syn (α-syn-mWT) (Touchman et al., 2001). Immunoblots demonstrated that 14 days treatment of primary neurons with α-syn-mWT pffs induced appearance of p-α-syn in the Tx-100-insoluble fraction (Figure 2B). Immunofluorescence also showed that α-syn-mWT pffs induced formation of p-α-syn aggregates in neurites and somata. Thus, pathological PD-like α-syn aggregates can be induced by α-syn-mWT pffs and does not require the human protein. Examination CP-673451 price of the α-syn aggregates using transmission and immuno-EM demonstrated abundant filaments in neurons treated with either α-syn-hWT or α-syn-1-120 pffs (Figure 3A) for 14 days, but not PBS-treated Carnitine dehydrogenase neurons (data

not shown). Remarkably, inclusions composed of 14- to 16-nm-thick filaments were seen throughout the cytoplasm, visualized by transmission EM. Two different immuno-EM detection systems, horse radish peroxidase (HRP) and immunogold amplification, were used to demonstrate that fibrils composed of p-α-syn are found throughout the neuron. P-α-syn-positive fibrils were seen in the soma (Figures 3B and 3C), adjacent to the active zone of presynaptic terminals (Figure 3D) and the postsynaptic terminal (Figure 3F) and throughout processes (Figure 3E). These data establish that the seeding and recruitment of endogenous mouse α-syn into hyperphosphorylated insoluble, filamentous aggregates recapitulate features of LBs and LNs in PD and other human synucleinopathies. To determine the temporal sequence of α-syn aggregate formation, α-syn-hWT pffs were added to the neurons at DIV5. P-α-syn immunostaining was not detectable until 4 days later when small aggregates began to appear, exclusively in the neurites, albeit at low levels (Figure 4A, upper series).

If a significant portion of the GFP−Zif+ neurons were extinction

If a significant portion of the GFP−Zif+ neurons were extinction neurons, then a negative correlation MDV3100 with freezing during extinction might be observed. On the other hand, if a significant portion of the GFP−Zif+ neurons were nontagged active fear neurons, then a positive correlation with freezing during extinction might be observed similar to the positive correlation found for GFP+Zif+ neurons ( Figure S1D). We did not find a significant correlation, either positive or negative ( Figure S5B). This suggests that GFP−Zif+ neurons might consist of a mix of neurons with varying

functions. Table S1 summarizes the extinction-induced perisomatic changes observed around the I-BET151 three types of BA neurons,

showing that the changes around GFP−Zif+ neurons differ from the changes around fear neurons, either silent (GFP+Zif−) or active (GFP+Zif+). The different perisomatic profiles around the three BA cell types illustrate the target-specific nature of fear extinction-induced perisomatic synapse remodeling. Our findings reveal that remodeling of perisomatic inhibitory synapses located immediately around fear neurons in the basal amygdala occurs during fear extinction. These perisomatic synapses represent a site where the circuits for fear extinction and fear storage connect. The direct anatomical and functional relationship between the perisomatic synapses and the fear neurons suggests a straightforward mechanism for the silencing of fear circuits. Perisomatic inhibitory synapses therefore provide an attractive therapeutic target for improving Adenylyl cyclase the efficacy of fear extinction in humans treated with exposure therapy. In addition, we found

that extinction might alter perisomatic inhibition outside of the fear circuit, possibly contributing to the behavioral effects of extinction by altering perisomatic inhibition of extinction neurons (Herry et al., 2008). The fine-tuned nature of the observed perisomatic synapse remodeling provides an important insight into how behavior can sculpt the flow of information in the brain. Notably, the extinction-induced remodeling of perisomatic synapses was interneuron and target-neuron specific, and the predicted changes in the balance of perisomatic inhibition matched the state of the target fear neurons in two ways (Figure 7). First, the silent state of fear neurons (GFP+Zif−) corresponded to an extinction-induced increase in perisomatic PV, which is predicted to increase perisomatic inhibition (Gittis et al., 2011 and Kohara et al., 2007). Second, the active state of fear neurons (GFP+Zif+) corresponded to an extinction-induced increase in perisomatic CB1R. We propose that the CB1R increase prevented a subset of active fear neurons from switching into silent fear neurons by decreasing GABA release from CCK terminals (Katona et al., 2001).

, 1997 and Currie et al , 1994) In subsequent replications (see

, 1997 and Currie et al., 1994). In subsequent replications (see below), a majority of layer-enriched NMDA receptor subunit genes were enriched in layers 2/3 (Table S6). Genes encoding proteins localized to the extracellular space or region were expressed at significantly higher levels in layers 2/3, 4, AZD6244 clinical trial 6, or 6b (Table S6). Surface markers in brain

cells are often involved in various signaling processes, from guidance to synapse formation (Maruyama et al., 2008, Uziel et al., 2006 and Yamamoto et al., 2007). We identified significant association of layer-enriched expression for genes whose human orthologs lie within genomic intervals previously associated with disease. In particular, mouse orthologs of human type 1 diabetes- and rheumatoid arthritis-associated genes were unusually abundant among layers 2/3-enriched genes (Figure 4C, Table S6). These findings reflected nearly all of these genes’ locations being within the major histocompatibility

complex (MHC) region. Indeed, genes in the MHC region were 34% more likely than randomly selected genes (p < 10−6; case resampling bootstrap) to have enriched expression in layers 2/3. Many of these examples are confirmed by in situ hybridizations in the Allen Mouse Brain Atlas (Figure S5). It was the nonimmune genes of the MHC region whose expression was particularly enriched no in layers 2/3 (Figure S5) and that

contributed to the significant selleck chemicals associations observed with these two diseases. In subsequent replications, a majority of layer-specific MHC genes and a large minority of all MHC genes were again enriched in layers 2/3 (Table S6). Another apparent disease association links mouse genes preferentially expressed in layer 5 with human genes in the Parkinson’s disease pathway (Figure 4B). This, however, is likely to reflect the involvement of mitochondrial dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease (Abou-Sleiman et al., 2006 and Burbulla et al., 2010) and the prominent expression of mitochondrial and metabolic genes in this layer (Table S5; Table S6). For example, Lrrk2, whose human ortholog is mutated in familial Parkinson’s disease ( Abou-Sleiman et al., 2006), is expressed prominently in rodent neocortical layers 2/3 and 5 (this study; Lein et al., 2007), specifically pyramidal neurons, and is associated with mitochondrial markers ( Biskup et al., 2006). In subsequent replications, nearly half of Parkinson’s disease-related genes were enriched in layers 2/3 and nearly half in layer 5 ( Table S6). Twenty-nine of the thirty-six genes with known enrichments from in situ hybridization were manually curated as enriched in layer 5 ( Table S6).